Wednesday, December 2, 2009

altruism

We May Be Born With an Urge to Help


By NICHOLAS WADE
Published: November 30, 2009
What is the essence of human nature? Flawed, say many theologians. Vicious and addicted to warfare, wrote Hobbes. Selfish and in need of considerable improvement, think many parents.
Skip to next paragraph
RSS Feed
• Get Science News From The New York Times »
Enlarge This Image

Sylvio Tuepke
Readers' Comments
Readers shared their thoughts on this article.
• Read All Comments (147) »
But biologists are beginning to form a generally sunnier view of humankind. Their conclusions are derived in part from testing very young children, and partly from comparing human children with those of chimpanzees, hoping that the differences will point to what is distinctively human.
The somewhat surprising answer at which some biologists have arrived is that babies are innately sociable and helpful to others. Of course every animal must to some extent be selfish to survive. But the biologists also see in humans a natural willingness to help.
When infants 18 months old see an unrelated adult whose hands are full and who needs assistance opening a door or picking up a dropped clothespin, they will immediately help, Michael Tomasello writes in “Why We Cooperate,” a book published in October. Dr. Tomasello, a developmental psychologist, is co-director of the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Leipzig, Germany.
The helping behavior seems to be innate because it appears so early and before many parents start teaching children the rules of polite behavior.
“It’s probably safe to assume that they haven’t been explicitly and directly taught to do this,” said Elizabeth Spelke, a developmental psychologist at Harvard. “On the other hand, they’ve had lots of opportunities to experience acts of helping by others. I think the jury is out on the innateness question.”
But Dr. Tomasello finds the helping is not enhanced by rewards, suggesting that it is not influenced by training. It seems to occur across cultures that have different timetables for teaching social rules. And helping behavior can even be seen in infant chimpanzees under the right experimental conditions. For all these reasons, Dr. Tomasello concludes that helping is a natural inclination, not something imposed by parents or culture.
Infants will help with information, as well as in practical ways. From the age of 12 months they will point at objects that an adult pretends to have lost. Chimpanzees, by contrast, never point at things for each other, and when they point for people, it seems to be as a command to go fetch something rather than to share information.
For parents who may think their children somehow skipped the cooperative phase, Dr. Tomasello offers the reassuring advice that children are often more cooperative outside the home, which is why parents may be surprised to hear from a teacher or coach how nice their child is. “In families, the competitive element is in ascendancy,” he said.
As children grow older, they become more selective in their helpfulness. Starting around age 3, they will share more generously with a child who was previously nice to them. Another behavior that emerges at the same age is a sense of social norms. “Most social norms are about being nice to other people,” Dr. Tomasello said in an interview, “so children learn social norms because they want to be part of the group.”
Children not only feel they should obey these rules themselves, but also that they should make others in the group do the same. Even 3-year-olds are willing to enforce social norms. If they are shown how to play a game, and a puppet then joins in with its own idea of the rules, the children will object, some of them vociferously.
Where do they get this idea of group rules, the sense of “we who do it this way”? Dr. Tomasello believes children develop what he calls “shared intentionality,” a notion of what others expect to happen and hence a sense of a group “we.” It is from this shared intentionality that children derive their sense of norms and of expecting others to obey them.
Shared intentionality, in Dr. Tomasello’s view, is close to the essence of what distinguishes people from chimpanzees. A group of human children will use all kinds of words and gestures to form goals and coordinate activities, but young chimps seem to have little interest in what may be their companions’ minds.
If children are naturally helpful and sociable, what system of child-rearing best takes advantage of this surprising propensity? Dr. Tomasello says that the approach known as inductive parenting works best because it reinforces the child’s natural propensity to cooperate with others. Inductive parenting is simply communicating with children about the effect of their actions on others and emphasizing the logic of social cooperation.
“Children are altruistic by nature,” he writes, and though they are also naturally selfish, all parents need do is try to tip the balance toward social behavior.
The shared intentionality lies at the basis of human society, Dr. Tomasello argues. From it flow ideas of norms, of punishing those who violate the norms and of shame and guilt for punishing oneself. Shared intentionality evolved very early in the human lineage, he believes, and its probable purpose was for cooperation in gathering food. Anthropologists report that when men cooperate in hunting, they can take down large game, which single hunters generally cannot do. Chimpanzees gather to hunt colobus monkeys, but Dr. Tomasello argues this is far less of a cooperative endeavor because the participants act on an ad hoc basis and do not really share their catch.
Skip to next paragraph
RSS Feed
• Get Science News From The New York Times »
Readers' Comments
Readers shared their thoughts on this article.
• Read All Comments (147) »
An interesting bodily reflection of humans’ shared intentionality is the sclera, or whites, of the eyes. All 200 or so species of primates have dark eyes and a barely visible sclera. All, that is, except humans, whose sclera is three times as large, a feature that makes it much easier to follow the direction of someone else’s gaze. Chimps will follow a person’s gaze, but by looking at his head, even if his eyes are closed. Babies follow a person’s eyes, even if the experimenter keeps his head still.
Advertising what one is looking at could be a risk. Dr. Tomasello argues that the behavior evolved “in cooperative social groups in which monitoring one another’s focus was to everyone’s benefit in completing joint tasks.”
This could have happened at some point early in human evolution, when in order to survive, people were forced to cooperate in hunting game or gathering fruit. The path to obligatory cooperation — one that other primates did not take — led to social rules and their enforcement, to human altruism and to language.
“Humans putting their heads together in shared cooperative activities are thus the originators of human culture,” Dr. Tomasello writes.
A similar conclusion has been reached independently by Hillard S. Kaplan, an anthropologist at the University of New Mexico. Modern humans have lived for most of their existence as hunter gatherers, so much of human nature has presumably been shaped for survival in such conditions. From study of existing hunter gatherer peoples, Dr. Kaplan has found evidence of cooperation woven into many levels of human activity.
The division of labor between men and women — men gather 68 percent of the calories in foraging societies — requires cooperation between the sexes. Young people in these societies consume more than they produce until age 20, which in turn requires cooperation between the generations. This long period of dependency was needed to develop the special skills required for the hunter gatherer way of life.
The structure of early human societies, including their “high levels of cooperation between kin and nonkin,” was thus an adaptation to the “specialized foraging niche” of food resources that were too difficult for other primates to capture, Dr. Kaplan and colleagues wrote recently in The Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. We evolved to be nice to each other, in other words, because there was no alternative.
Much the same conclusion is reached by Frans de Waal in another book published in October, “The Age of Empathy.” Dr. de Waal, a primatologist, has long studied the cooperative side of primate behavior and believes that aggression, which he has also studied, is often overrated as a human motivation.
“We’re preprogrammed to reach out,” Dr. de Waal writes. “Empathy is an automated response over which we have limited control.” The only people emotionally immune to another’s situation, he notes, are psychopaths.
Indeed, it is in our biological nature, not our political institutions, that we should put our trust, in his view. Our empathy is innate and cannot be changed or long suppressed. “In fact,” Dr. de Waal writes, “I’d argue that biology constitutes our greatest hope. One can only shudder at the thought that the humaneness of our societies would depend on the whims of politics, culture or religion.”
The basic sociability of human nature does not mean, of course, that people are nice to each other all the time. Social structure requires that things be done to maintain it, some of which involve negative attitudes toward others. The instinct for enforcing norms is powerful, as is the instinct for fairness. Experiments have shown that people will reject unfair distributions of money even it means they receive nothing.
“Humans clearly evolved the ability to detect inequities, control immediate desires, foresee the virtues of norm following and gain the personal, emotional rewards that come from seeing another punished,” write three Harvard biologists, Marc Hauser, Katherine McAuliffe and Peter R. Blake, in reviewing their experiments with tamarin monkeys and young children.
If people do bad things to others in their group, they can behave even worse to those outside it. Indeed the human capacity for cooperation “seems to have evolved mainly for interactions within the local group,” Dr. Tomasello writes.
Sociality, the binding together of members of a group, is the first requirement of defense, since without it people will not put the group’s interests ahead of their own or be willing to sacrifice their lives in battle. Lawrence H. Keeley, an anthropologist who has traced aggression among early peoples, writes in his book “War Before Civilization” that, “Warfare is ultimately not a denial of the human capacity for cooperation, but merely the most destructive expression of it.”
The roots of human cooperation may lie in human aggression. We are selfish by nature, yet also follow rules requiring us to be nice to others.
“That’s why we have moral dilemmas,” Dr. Tomasello said, “because we are both selfish and altruistic at the same time.”

7 comments:

  1. From reading the article, I believe that the altruistic actions that human children have innately have helped ensure the survival and proliferation of our race as a whole. I believe that many benefit can come out of cooperation that is strengthened by the innate altruistic nature. I believe that these innate qualities have evolved along with our sense of morality as well as our need to survive. In fact, this innate quality is a direct attack on Cultural Relativism. It shows that there are moral values that are innately sustained throughout different cultures.

    For example, lets say that in a culture, there is a moral rule saying that you must kill another individual, using this type of reasoning, that culture may not survive for many generations. Similarly, if there are moral rules saying that you must steal, the rivalry between individuals may be so high that it would be hard for such a culture to survive. Because of this, evolution dictates, that certain ethical rules be established in all cultures in order to allow them to survive. These encompassing rules directly go against the moral relativist's claim that there are no moral standards that can be applied to every culture; therefore there is no moral progress. In the case of the article, although young chimps do not have altruism innately, the fact that human babies do shows that there are certain moral qualities that we are born with as individuals; certain ethical traits that are shared before societal cultures can differentiate our ways of thinking. Thus, these qualities show that moral relativism may be flawed as a theory.

    ReplyDelete
  2. I really don't believe in the idea that humans are altruistic in nature. All I could think of when reading this article is how, when I was younger and my family used to eat together, I would be overcome by this feeling to be the first to get at the food and make sure that I got plenty of it before any one of my brothers. It made no sense now that I look back since there was always enough to go around and I usually didn't even eat all that I grabbed but the fact that the first emotion I felt was one of selfishness and self preservation and not sharing makes it hard for me to buy into the fact that we are all loving and peaceful.

    I believe that there is no explanation for our cooperation as a human race other than our selfishness. I agree with Dr. Kaplan's statement that we got along only because there was no other alternative. That right there says that any sense of cooperation, teamwork, or anything that has anything to do with getting along is driven by the fact that if we do not, we will not survive to see tomorrow. Every person wants certain things. In the case of the early humans, it was food. They knew that they needed food and that they could feed themselves better by hunting the bigger animals. What they did not know, at least before I'm sure a couple of them attempted it, was that they could not hunt the big game by themselves. So with the self interest of getting to eat the bigger game, they banned together into hunting groups and worked together to hunt the big game. The point is that even in working together, the driving factor was one of self.

    ReplyDelete
  3. This article is relevant to the discussion we were having in class a couple weeks ago, when we were trying to determine if people are born with an "innate moral compass." The research presented in this article is compelling in that it characterizes young children as helpful or willing to help. I agree that human beings are born with the capacity of being caring, helpful, and morally sound, but one has to question, who determined the standards that defined "moral sound" and are these the same morals for everyone. That is what I would like to know. This study was done in North America. Would babies from all over the globe do the same exact thing? I don't think so, I bet we could find a remote society somewhere out there, where babies do not help pick up safety pins. And I know what I am implying when I say this; that not all humans are born with the same innate morals, and that humans can somehow be genetically modified though years and years breeding within one culture in a captivated gene pool. I don't know. I think this study is not very thorough. We talked about multiple studies like this in Developmental Psychology, and the fact is that the Dev. psych field is torn on the issue: would every baby in every society behave similarly?

    ReplyDelete
  4. To really understand whether altruism is an innate trait, something nurtured, or whether it can ever really exist, we must first define it. Altruism is a motivational state with the ultimate goal of increasing another’s welfare. If we look at the “ultimate goal” part of this equation, we realize that it must be an end in itself and not just an intermediate for reaching a different goal. That is to say that if by helping another person, a child has any other goal (whether conscious or subconscious) than increasing the welfare of the recipient, then his or her act cannot be considered altruistic. It is quite possible that the child will help another person to either gain acceptance or positive regard, in which case the acts are actually egotistical. Furthermore, if a child acts to avoid guilt or shame, he or she is acting egotistically even if the action taken to prevent this discomfort was beneficial to another’s welfare. Even if seeing another person in distress causes the child distress and the child then acts in a way to relieve that distress, the act is not altruistic.
    The act itself could also be neither altruistic nor egotistical. If the ultimate goal is to uphold a principle or a duty, as directed or modeled by parents, the pursuit of this goal is not altruistic. Even if the adherence to the principle will benefit others, the goal is not altruistic because the beneficence of others is not the end of the goal itself. That is to say that if a child picks up a paperclip for a parent who has dropped it in order to do what they are “supposed” to do then the act cannot be altruistic because he or she does not have the welfare of another in mind.
    Since altruism is defined by intentions as opposed to consequences or actions, it is extremely difficult to know whether an act is altruistic or not. Something to take into consideration, however, is the evolutionary analysis of altruism. The type of fitness that Darwin referred to when he spoke of survival of the fittest (the term which was not actually coined by Darwin himself) referred solely to an animal’s ability to reproduce. Any trait that will help an animal reproduce will be carried on in greater extents than other traits by definition alone. This being said, an altruistic act in evolutionary terms, is an activity that promotes the fitness of the recipient at the expense of the provider. So, how could it ever be possible that a trait that decreases the fitness of an animal could actually survive over time? One possibility to consider is that showing gratitude or respect towards those that act altruistically ensures social success so recipients of altruism prosper by protecting those that act altruistically. Another possibility is that being seen as an altruistic being signals to potential mates the ability to raise the young best, thus aiding reproductive success. In both cases, the animal may have the ultimate goal to increase another’s welfare but, in turn, receives survival benefits.
    In this sense, we can see that it is possible that altruism could exist but it is impossible to prove that a person is acting altruistically.

    ReplyDelete
  5. This article shows the innate altruistic nature of children in revealing a young child’s instinctive actions to help an adult find what they have lost and follow the rules presented when playing with a group. I believe that every human is born with some moral compass and a desire to help others. In my own experience working at a daycare, it was often the case that if one child started to cry another child would as well. Or if a younger child began to cry an older one might offer their toy or blanket in what seemed an attempt to comfort them. If one child hurt their hand another would scream for an adult to help. I saw these acts as an innate sense of wanting to help each other, of feeling the pain of a friend crying and wanting to help comfort them in a time of pain.

    I feel that most people carry this altruistic feeling throughout their life. If we were not born with it and we did not practice a religion that promotes altruism, what would stop us from living a completely selfish life? Especially with the joy of giving that is all around us during the holiday season, I find it hard to believe that humans are born with only an innate desire to benefit themselves. Although, selfish desires must sometimes outweigh our desire to help others, I believe that an altruistic view is deeply ingrained in who we are as humans. This very well may have evolved because humans had to work together to survive and helping someone was ultimately helping the whole group. I think this evolution of altruistic action is a pivotal part of keeping our society functioning peacefully.
    -Olivia Thomas

    ReplyDelete
  6. This article shows the innate altruistic nature of children in revealing a young child’s instinctive actions to help an adult find what they have lost and follow the rules presented when playing with a group. I believe that every human is born with some moral compass and a desire to help others. In my own experience working at a daycare, it was often the case that if one child started to cry another child would as well. Or if a younger child began to cry an older one might offer their toy or blanket in what seemed an attempt to comfort them. If one child hurt their hand another would scream for an adult to help. I saw these acts as an innate sense of wanting to help each other, of feeling the pain of a friend crying and wanting to help comfort them in a time of pain.

    I feel that most people carry this altruistic feeling throughout their life. If we were not born with it and we did not practice a religion that promotes altruism, what would stop us from living a completely selfish life? Especially with the joy of giving that is all around us during the holiday season, I find it hard to believe that humans are born with only an innate desire to benefit themselves. Although, selfish desires must sometimes outweigh our desire to help others, I believe that an altruistic view is deeply ingrained in who we are as humans. This very well may have evolved because humans had to work together to survive and helping someone was ultimately helping the whole group. I think this evolution of altruistic action is a pivotal part of keeping our society functioning peacefully.
    -Olivia Thomas

    ReplyDelete
  7. I think that this idea of intrinsic altruism is a very interesting one. It is easy to see evidence of this because it is hard to find a truly evil person. I mean there may be people we did not like, maybe even people we thought were mean, but I can look back on my life and not think of someone who was truly evil. And I think that itself is signs of intrinsic altruism. As "racueva" discussed above, eating first was used as a counterpoint to intrinsic altruism. I don't agree with that because as stated there was plenty to eat, so one could afford to be selfish. This wasn't challenging the well being of another member of the family. -Joyce Ganas

    ReplyDelete